Science, Art, Litt, Science based Art & Science Communication
Q: Dr.Krishna, I have read your article on Nocebo Effect. But what about the placebo effect? Are placebo effects real?
Krishna: I will tell you what research says, not what people think about placebo effects.
A placebo could be pharmacologic (e.g., a tablet), physical (e.g., a manipulation), or psychological (e.g., a conversation).
An analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment was conducted (1).
Experts have concluded that reacting to a placebo is not proof that a certain treatment doesn't work, but rather that another, non-pharmacological mechanism may be present.
How placebos work is still not quite understood, but it involves a complex neurobiological reaction that includes everything from increases in feel-good neurotransmitters, like endorphins and dopamine, to greater activity in certain brain regions linked to moods, emotional reactions, and self-awareness. All of it can have therapeutic benefit. The placebo effect is a way for your brain to tell the body what it needs to feel better.
But placebos are not all about releasing brainpower. You also need the ritual of treatment. When you look at these studies that compare drugs with placebos, there is the entire environmental and ritual factor at work. You have to go to a clinic at certain times and be examined by medical professionals in white coats. You receive all kinds of exotic pills and undergo strange procedures. All this can have a profound impact on how the body perceives symptoms because you feel you are getting attention and care(2).
High-quality research shows arthroscopy to treat osteoarthritis, wear and tear of the meniscus in the knee, and to remove inflamed and thickened bone and tissue in the shoulder is no better than 'placebo surgery' (4).
But in studies testing surgical procedures, getting a placebo can mean going under the knife. So-called sham surgeries are a kind of extreme placebo, where patients undergo all the rituals and scars of a surgical procedure except for the part meant to help (5). These patients benefit surprisingly often: In about three-quarters of sham-controlled studies, there’s some improvement, according to a 2014 review. And they actually benefited just as much as those who got the actual intervention a whopping half the time. And therein lies perhaps the greatest contribution of sham surgeries: helping shine light on popular surgical interventions that might be shams themselves.
Placebo-controlled surgical trials are feasible and provide high-quality data on efficacy of surgical treatments. The surgical placebo entails a considerable risk for study participants. Consequently, a placebo should be used only if justified by the clinical question and by methodological necessity (6).
Placebos often work because people don't know they are getting one. But what happens if you know you are getting a placebo?
A study by researchers and published in Science Translational Medicine explored this by testing how people reacted to migraine pain medication. One group took a migraine drug labeled with the drug's name, another took a placebo labeled "placebo," and a third group took nothing. The researchers discovered that the placebo was 50% as effective as the real drug to make you feel a reduced pain after a migraine attack.
The researchers think that a driving force beyond this reaction was the simple act of taking a pill. People associate the ritual of taking medicine as a positive healing effect . Even if they know it's not medicine, the action itself can stimulate the brain into thinking the body is being healed.
But the reality is there is no actual healing!
A study published online by PLOS Biology may have identified what goes on in the brain during a placebo effect (3). Researchers used functional magnetic resonance imaging to scan the brains of people with chronic pain from knee osteoarthritis. Then everyone was given a placebo and had another brain scan. The researchers noticed that those who felt pain relief had greater activity in the middle frontal gyrus brain region, which makes up about one-third of the frontal lobe.
Scientists have come to recognize that there is not one response to a placebo but many. They refer to the sum total of patients’ responses to the administration of the placebo as the placebo response. This includes some proportion of the response that likely would have occurred in the absence of any treatment such as the natural history of a disease, regression to the mean, and response bias. Natural history denotes the change, up and down, of the symptoms over time. Regression to the mean refers to a statistical principle that if a variable is extreme on its first measurement, it will tend to be closer to the average on its second measurement, and vice versa. Response bias is the tendency for study participants to provide answers they believe investigators want to hear. The placebo effect is the proportion of improvement or worsening that remains after controlling for all the other incidental effects (7).
So it is all in the brain. It plays tricks with you! And makes you act like a fool!
Footnotes:
Tags:
101
The placebo effect is wild. Taking a sugar pill with no active medical ingredients can somehow trigger a psychedelic reaction in some people, if that's what they're expecting. Even more incredibly, placebos can work when people aren't being deceived too – when they know what they're taking isn't medication. In a new study, psychologists show this strange phenomenon can be leveraged as a simple way to reduce stress, at least in the short term for moderate levels of distress.
Exposure to long-term stress can impair a person's ability to manage emotions and cause significant mental health problems.
So it is interesting to see that an intervention that takes minimal effort can still lead to significant benefits.
MSU psychologist Darwin Guevarra and team prescribed a placebo for a group of 32 volunteers and offered no treatment to another group of 32 people.
The 64 participants all reported experiencing prolonged stress during the COVID-19 pandemic. Their stress levels, anxiety, and depression were measured before, in the middle, and at the end of the two-week trial.
The catch was, the placebo group knew they were getting pills with no active ingredients. They were instructed to take the inert plant fiber pills twice a day and had to fill out a pill-taking adherence survey daily.
Incredibly, those taking the placebo still had a decrease in stress, anxiety, and depression compared to the no-treatment group, despite knowing what they were taking was just a placebo.
As the sample size was small in this case, the researchers caution that more work needs to be done to see if the results hold true across different cultures and age groups and for longer time periods.
How this fascinating brain trickery works is not yet fully understood.
The data suggest that the effects of non-deceptive placebos on affective outcomes cannot be solely attributed to explicit expectations.
Other researchers have suggested that non-deceptive placebos may work through mechanisms such as implicit expectations, conditioning from prior experience with active treatment, and embodied cognition."
Whatever the exact mechanisms, the researchers suggest using a placebo to treat people experiencing moderate stress may help prevent them from deteriorating into more severe conditions.
"Remotely administered non-deceptive placebos have the potential to help individuals struggling with mental health concerns who otherwise would not have access to traditional mental health services," the researchers points out.
"This ability to administer non-deceptive placebos remotely increases scalability potential dramatically."
Other researchers argue there's not enough evidence to use placebos for treatments yet, as studies like these that support their effectiveness are small and too short-term.
This makes placebos perfect for use as a control in clinical trials to help eliminate biases, but more extensive research is required to confirm their therapeutic value.
https://iaap-journals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/aphw.12583
© 2024 Created by Dr. Krishna Kumari Challa. Powered by